Loading Add to favorites

Written by Christina Novelli, M.Ed., Doctoral Student, Department of Communication Sciences and Special Education, University of Georgia

Becoming literate, that is, possessing the ability to comprehend and produce messages in written text, is a primary purpose of formal schooling (Houghton & Zorzi, 2003). Proficient reading and writing skills are linked with positive school, work, and social life outcomes (Berman, 2009; DeWalt & Hink, 2009). Reading and writing are language-based behaviours, but unlike oral language development, reading and writing do not develop naturally (Moats, 2020). To learn the skills necessary to complete both tasks, students must learn to decode and encode units in English orthography.

English orthography is fundamentally—albeit not purely—alphabetic (Venezky, 1999). An alphabetic orthography (e.g., Hebrew) describes a system of written symbols (i.e., graphemes = letters or letter combinations) that represent individual phonemes (i.e., speech sounds). Understanding the alphabetic principle provides foundational knowledge for the structure of English. The alphabetic principle is comprised of two parts: (a) alphabetic understanding—knowing that words are made up of graphemes that represent phonemes and (b) phonological recoding—knowing how to translate the graphemes into their corresponding phonemes (Baker et al., 2018). In reading and writing development theories, phoneme-grapheme (encoding) and grapheme-phoneme (decoding) correspondences are underlying foundational skills (Kim, 2017; Kim & Schatschneider, 2017; Perfetti, 1992).

Early learners need to learn the basic connections between phonemes and graphemes and between graphemes and phonemes. Beginning readers see an unknown word (e.g., <clap>), break it up into parts, and use their knowledge of grapheme-phoneme correspondences to sound out the word (e.g., /k/ /l/ /æ/ /p/). Once readers pronounce the word, they use their semantic (i.e., meaning) system to comprehend it. As students become more proficient readers, they depend less on phonological processes because they have stored the word-specific representations in long-term memory. Conversely, beginning spellers translate sounds to print—they hear a spoken word (e.g., clap) and break it into parts (e.g., /k/ /l/ /æ/ /p/). Then they use their knowledge of phoneme-grapheme correspondences, letter formation, semantics, and fine motor skills to create the word's written form (McCloskey & Rapp, 2017). Much like reading, as students become more proficient spellers, spelling known words becomes automatic as the representations are stored in long-term memory (Ehri, 2005). Students with dyslexia experience more difficulties developing the automaticity of the connections necessary for fluent reading and spelling (Shaywitz et al., 2002). These students have difficulty hearing and manipulating the sounds of language, which is necessary for converting print-to-speech or speech-to-print.

Although reading and spelling share similar processing structures, spelling is more difficult because spellings represent all three linguistic structures (i.e., phonology, orthography, morphology). Since English is a morphophonemic system, the graphemes represent sound and mark the language's graphemic, syntactic, and morphemic features (Nunes & Bryant, 2006; Venezky, 1999). Spelling relies not only on basic phoneme-grapheme correspondences but also on context-sensitive rules. For example, the letter <c> is used to spell the sound /s/ when followed by an <e>, <i>, or <y>). Additionally, English spelling is composed of Greek, Latin, Anglo-Saxon, and Norman French elements and is therefore considered a deep orthography. Spelling has evolved to represent sound and meaning (Moats, 2020). For example, <bubble> is spelled with a doubled consonant to ensure the vowel sound is short, and this doubling pattern preserves a sound element. Spelling also can convey meaning in words. For example, in the word <helped>, the past-tense suffix <ed> is pronounced /t/; however, the suffix holds its spelling to reflect the meaning indicating the action already happened. If the word were spelled simply to preserve the sound element, it would be spelled <helpt>. Knowledge of all three linguistic structures and their interactions, or connections, is paramount to successful reading and writing development.

Sound Walls are Tools to Support Connections

When instruction is well-designed and strategic, learning for all students improves. A high-leverage practice for students with disabilities is to provide scaffolded supports (McLeskey et al., 2017). These include visual, verbal, and written supports, which are intended to temporarily support students to successfully engage in behaviours they may not have full mastery of yet. A sound wall is a visual scaffold to help students solidify their knowledge of phoneme-grapheme correspondences. In their synthesis of spelling interventions, Galuschka et al. (2020) found spelling interventions that included phonics training, explicit instruction of spelling rules, and morphological instruction were particularly effective. Sound walls support the first two components needed by providing a visual support to students to strengthen their knowledge of phonics and spelling patterns.

consonant sound wall

Click here to view and download Consonant Sound Wall Information. 

Vowel sound wall

Click here to view and download Vowel Sound Wall Information.

Using Sound Walls in Coordination with Intervention

Although sound walls can support all students, they may be especially important tools for students with dyslexia. Reading and spelling interventions conducted in a one-on-one or small group setting are effective for students with learning disabilities (Galuschka et al., 2020). Sound walls can be incorporated into small-group instruction. Students can have a personal reference copy by using a file folder and displaying each sound wall on an inside flap. The teacher can choose for these individualized sound walls to be constructed after instruction or revealed during instruction. Students can write in the graphemes as they learn additional spellings or can highlight learned graphemes on a completed sound wall.

It is important for educators to use data to determine an appropriate entry point in the scope and sequence. This can be done through diagnostic assessments using criterion-referenced tests (e.g., Gallistel-Ellis Test of Coding Skills; Gallistel, 2005) or teacher-made mastery measurement assessments of foundational reading and spelling skills (e.g., reading and spelling of taught sounds). These data and information from the student’s individual education plan will point to a place to begin in the scope and sequence. Small-group instruction, like whole-group instruction, will be explicit and systematic and follow explicit instruction (see Table 1 for example script).

Table 1

Introducing a New Phoneme and Its Most Common Spelling

Step Teacher Behavior Example Phrasing Student Behavior
1 Model target sound “Listen /f/. Your turn, /f/” Students imitate
2 Direct students to look at your mouth when producing the target sound “Look at my mouth” Students observe teacher
3 Model target sound again “Listen /f/. Your turn, /f/” Students imitate
4 Elicit responses about what the lips, tongue, and vocal cords are doing while making the target sound. “Where is your tongue? Where are your teeth? Are your vocal cords noisy or quiet?” Students respond to questions
5 Introduce mouth picture. “This is what your mouth looks like when we say /f/”  
6 Model target sound again “Look and listen, /f/. Your turn /f/” Students imitate
7 Introduce grapheme that spells the target sound “f [show grapheme] represents /f/. Your turn, /f/” Students repeat
8 Model how to form grapheme “Watch me as I spell /f/. Your turn spell /f/” Students practice
9 Add (or reveal) a mouth picture, sound card, and letter card to the sound wall    

In addition, formative assessment can guide instructional decision-making (move on or re-teach). One practice is to incorporate a daily spelling check after each intervention session. Words used for the daily check need to reflect previously and recently taught phoneme-grapheme correspondences. For the check, the teacher dictates five sounds, and the student(s) write all known spellings without the assistance of their sound wall. This is to monitor independent mastery of the spellings. For students progressing in orthographic learning, the dictation may switch from sounds to words (i.e., the teacher dictates words encompassing the different spelling rules learned). The check procedure follows the following script for each sound.

T: “The sound is /sound/. What sound?”

S: Imitate sound.

T: “Spell /sound/. We know XX spellings so far.”

S: Write learned graphemes.

There is no corrective feedback during the spell check as it is a formative assessment, but general praise for staying on-task and completing the sound check should be provided. Additionally, teachers can have students practice self-monitoring by actively scoring their checks using their personal sound walls.

Summary

Sound walls are scaffolds that pair phonological and orthographic information to support students’ speech-to-print connections. Awareness of articulatory gestures—what the mouth looks like during phoneme production—enables students to connect to the motor movements of phonemes to graphemes. By incorporating sound walls within intervention, teachers increase the likelihood of students successfully applying speech-to-print connections while writing.

About the Author:

Christina Novelli is currently pursuing her Ph.D. in special education at the University of Georgia. She is also a National Center for Leadership in Intensive Intervention scholar. Her research interests include the intersection of quality reading, spelling, and writing assessment and instruction, particularly for students with or at risk for word-level reading disabilities. Additionally, Christina is interested in the continued application of the principles of applied behaviour analysis to academic intervention and data-based decision-making.

References

Baker, S. K., Santiago, R. T., Masser, J., Nelson, N. J., & Turtura, J. (2018). The alphabetic

principle: From phonological awareness to reading words. Washington, DC: U. S. Retrieved from https://improvingliteracy.org.

Berman, I. (2009, February 25). Supporting adolescent literacy achievement [Issue brief]. Washington, DC: National Governors Association Center for Best Practices.

DeWalt, D. A., & Hink, A. (2009). Health literacy and child health outcomes: A systematic

review of the literature. Pediatrics124, S265–S274.

Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–188.

Gallistel, E. (2005). Gallistel-Ellis Test of Coding Skills. Montage Press.

Galuschka, K., Görgen, R., Kalmar, J., Haberstroh, S., Schmalz, X., & Schulte-Körne. (2020). Effectiveness of spelling interventions for learners with dyslexia: A meta-analysis and systematic review. Educational Psychologist, 55(1), 1–20.

Houghton, G., & Zorzi, M. (2003). Normal and impaired spelling in a connectionist dual-route architecture. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(2), 115–162.

Kim, Y.-S. G. (2017). Why the simple view of reading is not simplistic: Unpacking component skills of reading using a direct and indirect effect model of reading (DIER). Scientific Studies of Reading, 21(4), 310–333.

Kim, Y.-S. G., & Schatschneider, C. (2017). Expanding the developmental models of writing: A direct and indirect effects model of developmental writing (DIEW). Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(1), 35–50.

McCloskey, M. & Rapp, B. (2017). Developmental dysgraphia: An overview and framework for research. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 34(3–4), 65–82.

Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (2006). Improving literacy by teaching morphemes. Routledge.

Perfetti, C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, L. C.

Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 145–174). Erlbaum.

Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P.,

Constable, R. T., Marchione, K. E., Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., & Gore, J. C. (2002). Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Society of Biological Psychiatry, 52, 101–110.

Venezky, R. L. (1999). The American way of spelling: The structure and origins of American English orthography. Guilford.

Table References

Archer, A. L., & Hughes, C. A. (2011). Explicit instruction: Effective and efficient teaching. Guilford.

Baker, S. K., Santiago, R. T., Masser, J., Nelson, N. J., & Turtura, J. (2018). The alphabetic

principle: From phonological awareness to reading words. Washington, DC: U. S. Retrieved from https://improvingliteracy.org.

Berman, I. (2009, February 25). Supporting adolescent literacy achievement [Issue brief]. Washington, DC: National Governors Association Center for Best Practices.

Boyer, N. & Ehri, L. C. (2011). Contribution of phonemic segmentation instruction with letters and articulation pictures to word reading and spelling in beginners. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(5), 440–470.

DeWalt, D. A., & Hink, A. (2009). Health literacy and child health outcomes: A systematic

review of the literature. Pediatrics124, S265–S274.

Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–188.

Fälth, L., Gustafson, S., & Svensson, I. (2017). Phonological awareness training with articulation

promotes early reading development. Education, 137(3), 261–276.

Galazka, M. A., Hadijikhani, N., Sundqvist, M., Johnels, J. Å. (2021) Facial speech processing in

children with and without dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 71, 501–524.

Gallistel, E. (2005). Gallistel-Ellis Test of Coding Skills. Montage Press.

Galuschka, K., Görgen, R., Kalmar, J., Haberstroh, S., Schmalz, X., & Schulte-Körne. (2020). Effectiveness of spelling interventions for learners with dyslexia: A meta-analysis and systematic review. Educational Psychologist, 55(1), 1–20.

Houghton, G., & Zorzi, M. (2003). Normal and impaired spelling in a connectionist dual-route architecture. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(2), 115–162.

Kim, Y.-S. G. (2017). Why the simple view of reading is not simplistic: Unpacking component skills of reading using a direct and indirect effect model of reading (DIER). Scientific Studies of Reading, 21(4), 310–333.

Kim, Y.-S. G., & Schatschneider, C. (2017). Expanding the developmental models of writing: A direct and indirect effects model of developmental writing (DIEW). Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(1), 35–50.

Lindamood, P. C. & Lindamood, P. D. (2011). The Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing© program

for reading, spelling, and speech: Manuel. (4th ed). Pro-Ed.

McCloskey, M. & Rapp, B. (2017). Developmental dysgraphia: An overview and framework for research. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 34(3–4), 65–82.

McLeskey, J., Barringer, M.-D., Billingsley, B., Brownell, M., Jackson, D., Kennedy, M., Lewis, T., Maheady, L., Rodriguez, J., Scheeler, M. C., Winn, J., & Ziegler, D. (2017, January). High-leverage practices in special education. Council for Exceptional Children & CEEDAR Center.

Moats, L. C. (2020). Speech to print: Language essentials for teachers. Brookes Publishing.

National Reading Panel (U. S.), & National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (U. S.). (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. U. S. Dept. of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

Novelli, C., & Ardoin, S. P. (2023). Seeing the mouth: The importance of articulatory gestures during phonics training. [Manuscript under review].

Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (2006). Improving literacy by teaching morphemes. Routledge.

Perfetti, C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, L. C.

Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 145–174). Erlbaum.

Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P.,

Constable, R. T., Marchione, K. E., Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., & Gore, J. C. (2002). Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Society of Biological Psychiatry, 52, 101–110.

Torgesen, J., Alexander, A. W., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Voeller, K. K. S., & Conway, T. (2001). Intensive remedial instruction for children with severe reading disabilities: Immediate and long-term outcomes from two instructional approaches. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 33–58.

Venezky, R. L. (1999). The American way of spelling: The structure and origins of American English orthography. Guilford.

Williams, K. J., Walker, M. A., Vaughn, S., & Wanzek, J. (2017). A synthesis of reading and spelling interventions and their effects on spelling outcomes for students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 50(3), 286–297.